Open Access
31 May 2021 Correlating functional near-infrared spectroscopy with underlying cortical regions of 0-, 1-, and 2-year-olds using theoretical light propagation analysis
Author Affiliations +
Abstract

Significance: The establishment of a light propagation analysis-based scalp-cortex correlation (SCC) between the scalp location of the source–detector (SD) pair and brain regions is essential for measuring functional brain development in the first 2 years of life using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS).

Aim: We aimed to reveal the optics-based SCC of 0-, 1-, and 2-year-olds (yo) and the suitable SD distance for this age period.

Approach: Light propagation analyses using age-appropriate head models were conducted on SD pairs at 10-10 fiducial points on the scalp to obtain optics-based SCC and its metrics: the number of corresponding brain regions (NCBR), selectivity and sensitivity of the most likely corresponding brain region (MLCBR), and consistency of the MLCBR across developmental ages. Moreover, we assessed the suitable SD distances for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo by simultaneously considering the selectivity and sensitivity of the MLCBR.

Results: Age-related changes in the SCC metrics were observed. For instance, the NCBR of 0-yo was larger than that of 1- and 2-yo. Conversely, the selectivity of 0-yo was lower than that of 1- and 2-yo. The sensitivity of 1-yo was higher than that of 0-yo at 15- to 30-mm SD distances and higher than that of 2-yo at 10-mm SD distance. Notably, the MLCBR of the fiducial points around the longitudinal fissure was inconsistent across age groups. An SD distance between 15 and 25 mm was found to be appropriate for satisfying both sensitivity and selectivity requirements. In addition, this work provides reference tables of optics-based SCC for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo.

Conclusions: Optics-based SCC will be informative in designing and explaining child developmental studies using fNIRS. The suitable SD distances were between 15 and 25 mm for the first 2 years of life.

1.

Introduction

The first two years of human life are characterized by the most dynamic growth in brain structures15 and remarkable cognitive and behavioral changes.6,7 Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS; a list of abbreviations is provided in Table S1 in the Supplementary Material for the convenience of the reader) is an irreplaceable neuroimaging tool for studying early brain functional development, providing unprecedented opportunities for recording the hemodynamic response in awake, behaving infants because of its balanced temporal-spatial resolution and resilience to movement.811 Notably, although the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention defines 0- to 1-year-olds (yo) and 2- to 3-yo children as infants and toddlers, respectively, we have described 0- to 2-yo children as infants in this study for readability.

Despite the suitability of fNIRS in infant studies, a major limitation of this technique is the inability of fNIRS data to provide structural information of the head tissue. In fNIRS measurements, a source–detector (SD) pair positioned on the scalp surface measures local concentration changes in oxygenated and deoxygenated hemoglobin caused by neural activity.1215 On the other hand, while neural activity tied to a specific human function originates in local brain regions. The absence of structural information in the fNIRS signal makes it impossible to correlate the signal response with the anatomical brain regions. Therefore, in fNIRS studies, the scalp location where the SD pair is attached to its underlying brain region where the fNIRS signal originates should be mapped. We call this mapping the scalp-cortex correlation (SCC).

Several methods for obtaining SCC have been proposed for studies on adults; however, only a few studies have provided SCC for the infant population. Similar to adults, infant SCC is mostly based on a simple geometrical technique, i.e., correlating the location of the SD pair on the scalp, typically, the midpoint of the SD pair, with cortical regions in a simple point-to-point geometrical manner. For instance, researchers often referred to the international 10-20 or 10-10 system16 when attaching SD pairs on the scalp and then inferred the anatomical locations1719 according to the geometrical SCC of the adult head20,21 or infant head.2225 Notably, by linearly reducing the size of the adult heads, the virtual registration method26 has also been employed to estimate SCC in infant studies.2729

As described above, the point-to-point geometrical SCC provides a tolerable estimation of the underlying brain regions for the absorption change acquired by the SD pair. Nevertheless, the geometrical SCC is based on the assumption that the absorption change occurs at the cortical projection point below the midpoint between the SD pair, and light scattering in the head tissue is not considered. Mounting evidence from light propagation analysis in the adult head revealed light scattering in the head tissue could have a considerable influence on the partial pathlength (PPL) in the brain and the spatial sensitivity profile (SSP).3034 Notably, a few studies have already demonstrated that light propagation in the infant heads is distinct from that in adults owing to structural differences.35,36 Furthermore, very recent studies on adults have started considering light propagation in turbid media when calculating the SCC.37,38 However, to date, no light propagation analysis-based SCC data are available for 0- to 2-yo infants. In addition to age, SD distance must have a significant influence on optics-based SCC during early development. Only a few studies have examined the effect of SD distance on fNIRS sensitivity in infant brain tissue.35,39 For example, Fukui et al.35 found that fNIRS sensitivity to gray matter (GM) and white matter (WM) of neonates was modulated by the SD distance. These threads of evidence revealed that it remains largely unknown how age and SD distance affect optics-based SCC in 0- to 2-yo infants and how to choose an appropriate SD distance to ensure both the sensitivity to cerebral hemodynamics and the selectivity of signals from a specific brain region of interest. To address these issues, the current study aimed to create a precise optics-based SCC between SD pairs on the scalp fiducial point and brain regions defined by a macro-anatomical atlas by considering the light scattering in 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infant heads. In addition, we quantitatively characterized the changes in SCC with age and SD distance and evaluated the suitable SD distances for each age. The optics-based SCC was obtained for each SD pair by solving the diffusion equation.

2.

Materials and Methods

2.1.

Infant Head Structure and AAL Atlas

The anatomical head structure of the infants and the corresponding brain atlas used in this study were obtained from publicly available data.40 In the present study, we used age-appropriate average structural images acquired with a 3T magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) scanner. A set of longitudinal images of 95 healthy infants [56 males and 39 females, gestational age at birth: 37.9±1.8 (mean ± standard deviation) weeks] were scanned three times when their postmenstrual age was 41.5±1.7, 94.2±3.4, and 146.2±4.9 weeks, respectively. Based on the difference in subtracting gestational age from postmenstrual age, the population used in this study could be divided into three age groups concentrating around 0, 1, and 2 years of age. All participants in our dataset had normal fetal ultrasound during pregnancy and were free of congenital anomalies, metabolic disease, and focal lesions after birth. T2-weighted images were obtained with a voxel size of 1.25×1.25×1.95  mm3 for 0-yo and T1-weighted images were obtained with a voxel size of 1×1×1  mm3 for 1- and 2-yo. The tissue probability maps of GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) that exhibited similar geometry to the structural images were also used for head tissue segmentation (see Sec. 2.2.1).

To obtain the optics-based SCC between the fiducial points and brain regions, we chose the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas41 to parcellate the infant’s brain. The AAL atlas is widely used in cognitive neuroscience. In addition, remarkably, it parcellates a human brain into multiple non-overlapping regions according to the identification of main sulci, which are already clearly visible from birth and preserved throughout normal brain development.22 We used the AAL atlas with 90 brain regions (Table S2 in the Supplementary Material) in the infant space, which maintains the consistency of the AAL map propagation from the adult Colin 27 brain to the infant images using indirect fusion approach and a feature-based groupwise registration algorithm (see Ref. 40). Infant atlases from 0-, 1-, and 2-yo were built using infant MRI segmentation and groupwise registration methods. The atlases are publicly available on the NITRC website.42

2.2.

Light Propagation Analysis

2.2.1.

Construction of age-appropriate structural head models

We can use an age-appropriate template of the head to substitute the subject-specific head anatomy to localize the macroanatomical structure when individual infant MRIs are not available.24,25 Hence, we constructed three age-appropriate head models by segmenting the tissues of average MRI images from 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants for further light propagation analysis. Specifically, a semiautomatic approach was used to segment the MRI images into air and five types of head tissues with different optical properties. First, the head masks, i.e., the air/scalp boundaries, were extracted from the average T2-weighted images for 0-yo, and T1-weighted images for 1- and 2-yo using a simple thresholding method. Second, the tissue probability maps of the CSF, GM, and WM were converted to binary intracranial regions in each image. It was difficult to identify the scalp/skull and skull/CSF boundaries in the average MRI images because of the thin structures of the scalp and skull and blurring caused by the averaging of slightly misaligned multi-subject images. Thus, we applied morphological operations to extract the scalp/skull and skull/CSF boundaries based on the representative thickness of the scalp (3.5, 4, and 4 mm for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo, respectively) and skull (2.2, 3, and 3.8 mm for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo, respectively), as described in the literature.43,44 Finally, five-layered head models of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants were created by integrating the above intracranial regions and boundaries of superficial tissues. Contradictions in the integration process were manually and/or automatically corrected. The age-appropriate five-layered models for the three age groups are shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Age-appropriate five-layered head models for (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo infants, which comprise the scalp, skull, CSF, GM, and WM. The first, second, and third columns show the sagittal, axial, and coronal views of the head models, respectively.

NPH_8_2_025009_f001.png

2.2.2.

Light propagation calculation and optical property

The volumetric tetrahedral mesh for each age-appropriate head model was generated using the iso2mesh toolbox45 for light propagation analysis by employing the diffusion equation along with the finite element method. Similar to a previous study,46 we confirmed that the quality of the created volumetric tetrahedral mesh was sufficient for conducting the light propagation analysis. Specifically, we calculated the number of nodes, elements, and faces in each mesh model and further computed the Joe–Lie quality index,47 qvol, for every tetrahedron for all three ages using the following equation:

qvol=12×(3×vol)230ij3li,j2,
where vol is the tetrahedral volume, and li,j are the lengths of the edges of the tetrahedron. This metric is equal to 1 for equilateral tetrahedra and tends to zero for degenerated tetrahedra. The higher the qvol value, the higher the quality of the mesh. In Table 1, we report the total number (N) of nodes, elements, and faces. The mean qvol (across all tetrahedrons) with its standard deviation is also shown. We also found the majority of volumetric tetrahedral meshes for each age group had a high Joe–Liu quality value (76.9%, 80.5%, and 79.2% of all meshes have Joe–Liu quality values higher than 0.7 for 0- to 2-yo infants). Light propagation in the head models was calculated using the Nirfast software,48 a finite element-based package that uses the diffusion approximation for modeling near-infrared light transport in tissue.49,50 The optical properties of each tissue type in the infant head models for a wavelength of 800 nm were specified as in previous studies,35,51 namely, the absorption coefficient μa, scattering coefficient μs, anisotropy factor g, and refractive index n (Table 2). Notably, the reduced scattering coefficient μs should be used to analyze light propagation using the diffusion equation, where μs=μs(1g).

Table 1

Properties of the volumetric mesh (number of nodes, elements, faces, and the Joe–Liu quality index) for every age.

0-yo1-yo2-yo
N nodes101056212418243541
N elements59424112608961451619
N faces293208519898555656
Mean qvol±std0.784±0.1310.799±0.1230.793±0.127

Table 2

Optical properties of tissue types for light propagation analysis.

μa (mm−1)μs (mm−1)gn
Scalp0.018190.91.4
Skull0.016160.91.4
CSF0.00410.320.91.4
GM0.0485.00.91.4
WM0.037100.91.4

2.2.3.

Fiducial points and arrangement of SD pairs

The international 10-10 system positions were virtually set on the age-appropriate head models of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo using custom analysis scripts written in MATLAB (MathWorks, Natick, Massachusetts). First, we manually identified the four anatomical landmarks, including the inion, nasion, and left and right periauricular points on the scalp surface of the infant head models. Then, 61 fiducial points of the 10-10 system16,52 were automatically assigned to the scalp of the head model of every age, as depicted in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2

Superimposed anatomical landmarks (in dark green), 10-20 fiducial points (in yellow), and 10-10 fiducial points (in white) on the head models of (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. For each row panel, five views of the head models are displayed.

NPH_8_2_025009_f002.png

Because the SD distance significantly affects the sampling regions, the distances between sources and detectors were set as 10, 15, 20, 25, and 30 mm, where the midpoints of the SD pairs were set at the 10-10 fiducial points. Two SD pairs were placed according to the circumferential and vertical orientations at each fiducial point to examine whether the probe orientation influences the SCC for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo. The SD pair arrangements on the head models for five different SD distances for each age are shown in Fig. 3.

Fig. 3

SD pair arrangements on head models for five different SD distances for (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. The sources and detectors are indicated in red and blue dots, respectively. Two orientational SD pairs at the 10-20 fiducial points (yellow dots) are displayed only on the right hemisphere. Notably, SD pairs were also attached to the 10-10 fiducial points (white dots); however, the sources and detectors at these locations are not shown to avoid complications. The circumferential and vertical SD pairs at each fiducial point are indicated by two dashed black connections, respectively.

NPH_8_2_025009_f003.png

2.2.4.

Calculation of photon measurement density function and normalized PPL

Similar to our previous adult study,38 we adopted the optics-based method to analyze SCC. Based on the results of the light propagation analysis, the photon measurement density function (PMDF), which has the same spatial distribution as SSP,53 was calculated at each node of the mesh model.54 As the sum of the SSPs is equivalent to the PPL in a particular brain region, the PMDF sum is linearly related to the PPL for that region. Given that each node in the mesh model was labeled as a specific AAL brain region, we could define the normalized PPL (Lnorm,M) in a given brain region M using the following equation to quantify the SCC:

Lnorm,M=lMΣj=1Nlj,
where lM and lj are the sum of the PMDFs of all nodes within the brain regions M and j, respectively. N is the total number of brain regions within the whole brain tissue. According to the equation, Lnorm,M ranges from 0 to 1. Because the PMDF is a probability density function,55 we could use Lnorm,M as an index to represent the probability that the fNIRS signal is affected by the brain activation of region M. We calculated Lnorm,M of two orientational SD pairs (circumferential and vertical orientations) placed at every fiducial point for the 0-, 1-, and 2-yo head models.

2.3.

Characterization of SCC

2.3.1.

Evaluation metrics

For fNIRS users who are involved in developmental neuroscience, the following four questions are foremost: (1) how many brain regions are associated with an SD pair; (2) which brain region is the most likely corresponding brain region (MLCBR) for a given SD pair and its probability; (3) whether the sensitivity is sufficient to measure brain activity in the MLCBR, and (4) whether the MLCBR for the same scalp location is consistent across early development. Therefore, we defined four metrics, namely, the number of corresponding brain regions (NCBR), the selectivity of the MLCBR, the sensitivity of the MLCBR, and the consistency of the MLCBR across physical development, to further characterize the SCC and to systematically investigate the effects of age and SD distance on the SCC.

The detailed definitions of the four metrics are described below. Due to the strong scattering of near-infrared light passing through head tissues, a fiducial point usually projects to more than one brain region. Hence, we simply counted the number of brain regions that were correlated to a given fiducial point as the NCBR. The current study used Lnorm,M=0.05, as a threshold to calculate NCBR. Moreover, we chose the brain region with the largest Lnorm,M, as the MLCBR for a given fiducial point. Lnorm,MLCBR were defined as the metrics of selectivity at the fiducial point. For the sensitivity metric, we calculated the absolute PPL of MLCBR. The absolute PPL in the MLCBR was obtained using the following equation:

Labs,MLCBR=ln(Ibase/Ipert)0.001μa,MLCBR,
where Ibase and Ipert are the detected intensities when the absorption coefficient of the MLCBR (Δμa,MLCBR) is the baseline and perturbed states (0.1% increase), respectively. Finally, to examine whether the MLCBR at the same fiducial point was consistent from 0- to 2-yo, all fiducial points were classified into five categories according to consistency: (1) completely consistent, i.e., the MLCBR was the same for the brain region among the three ages; (2) consistent between 0- and 1-yo; (3) consistent between 0- and 2-yo; (4) consistent between 1- and 2-yo; and (5) inconsistent between any age groups.

2.3.2.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using R. Almost all analyses were performed using R version 3.6.3 in R Studio Version 1.2.5033 (RStudio Team, 2019). The following packages were used for data manipulation, visualization, and statistical tests: dplyr v1.0.2, tidyverse v1.3.0, rstatix v0.6.0, magrittr v1.5, nparLD v2.1, and ggpubr v0.4.0. Only NCBR was assessed with R 3.5.1, using the nparLD package. The NCBR, selectivity, and sensitivity values at each 10-10 fiducial point (61 in total) were treated as dependent variables. Provided that the dependent variable NCBR deviated from normality and equal variability, we applied a rank-based non-parametric mixed model statistical method, nparLD56 with an F1-LD-F1 design, to investigate the effects of age and SD distance on NCBR. We reported the Wald-type statistic (WTS) to assess the statistical significance of age, SD distance, and their interaction. If the interaction or main effects were significant, comparisons between two conditions were conducted using the nonparametric Mann-Whitney U test (independent samples) or Wilcoxon signed-rank test (dependent samples), followed by the Bonferroni method for multiple comparison adjustment. For the dependent variables of sensitivity or selectivity, a mixed-design ANOVA was conducted to examine the effects of age and SD distance. Specifically, the sensitivity and selectivity were subjected to two two-way mixed ANOVAs with the SD distance (10, 15, 20, 25, and 30 mm) as a within-subjects factor and age (0-, 1-, and 2-yo) as a between-subjects factor. If the interaction or main effects were significant, comparisons between two conditions were conducted using a two-sample t-test (independent samples) or paired t-test (dependent samples), followed by the Bonferroni method for multiple comparison adjustment. In all ANOVA analyses, Greenhouse–Geisser corrections were applied on violation of the sphericity assumption. The generalized eta squared57,58 served as estimates of the effect sizes.

3.

Results

3.1.

Optics-Based SCC for a Representative Fiducial Point

To illustrate the changes in SSP due to SD distance and age, the PMDF of the circumferential SD pair at the fiducial point T4 is shown in Fig. 4. The spatial distribution of the PMDF broadened as the SD distance increased, regardless of age. The values of Lnorm,M higher than 0.01, at the fiducial point T4 are shown in Table 3. According to the definition of the NCBR (i.e., the number of brain regions whose Lnorm,M was >0.05), NCBR was 2 at the fiducial point T4, which depicts the right middle and inferior temporal gyrus (MTG-R and ITG-R, respectively). These two brain regions had dominant Lnorm,M at the fiducial point T4 for any age and SD distance (indicated in boldface in Table 3), where the MLCBR was MTG_R. In addition to MTG_R and ITG_R, other brain regions, the superior temporal gyrus (STG-R) and right temporal pole of the superior and middle temporal gyrus (TPOsup-R and TPOmid-R, respectively), were also associated with T4, and their Lnorm,M increased as the SD distance increased. Considering the STG_R of 0-yo as an example, T4 had a Lnorm,STG_R of 0.009 for a 10-mm SD distance, whereas the Lnorm,STG_R reached 0.022 when the SD distance was 30 mm (see Table 3). For the same SD distance, the Lnorm,MTG_R of 1- or 2-yo was greater than that of 0-yo. For instance, the Lnorm,MTG_R values for a 20-mm SD distance were 0.593, 0.786, and 0.861 for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo, respectively. The PMDF of the vertical SD pair set at fiducial point T4 is shown in Fig. S1 in the Supplementary Material. Likewise, we found that the vertical SD pair at T4 was mainly correlated with the two brain regions, that is, MTG_R and ITG_R. The maximum Lnorm,STG_R of the vertical SD pair was only 0.060, 0.070, and 0.094 for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo, respectively (SD distance=30  mm). As shown in Table 3, the fiducial point T4 had the same MLCBR, that is, MTG_R, for any age and SD distance; therefore, the MLCBR was completely consistent among the three ages. The sensitivity of MLCBR (Labs,MLCBR) is also shown in Table 3. We also observed an increase in the Labs,MLCBR at each age with increasing SD distance. The Labs,MLCBR of 0-yo was smaller than that of 1- and 2-yo, while Labs,MLCBR of 1-yo was almost similar or slightly larger than that of 2-yo.

Fig. 4

PMDF for a given fiducial point T4 at five SD distances for 0-yo, 1-yo, and 2-yo when the SD pair was attached in a circumferential orientation. Dashed lines in different colors indicate the AAL brain region boundaries. The PMDF superimposed on age-appropriate brain structures of (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo.

NPH_8_2_025009_f004.png

Table 3

Normalized PPL of corresponding brain regions (Lnorm,M) and the sensitivity of MLCBR (Labs,MLCBR) for the circumferential SD pair set at the fiducial point T4 at five SD distances for 0-yo, 1-yo, and 2-yo.

AgeBrain regionSD distance
10 mm15 mm20 mm25 mm30 mm
0-yoMTG_R0.6360.6510.5930.6110.562
ITG_R0.3460.3180.3660.3320.366
STG_R0.0090.0130.0140.0210.022
TPOsup_R0.0040.0070.0090.0130.017
TPOmid_R0.0030.0060.0100.0130.020
Labs,MLCBR (mm)1.94.65.97.99.1
1-yoMTG_R0.8360.8180.7860.7860.746
ITG_R0.1530.1680.1960.1880.224
STG_R0.0090.0110.0140.0200.022
TPOsup_R0.0010.0010.0010.0020.003
TPOmid_R0.0000.0000.0010.0010.002
Labs,MLCBR (mm)4.66.28.010.411.3
2-yoMTG_R0.8900.8690.8610.8240.819
ITG_R0.0780.0970.0940.1240.117
STG_R0.0260.0270.0340.0370.046
TPOsup_R0.0020.0030.0040.0070.009
TPOmid_R0.0010.0010.0010.0030.003
Labs,MLCBR (mm)3.95.87.69.811.3
Note: The Lnorm,M value higher than 0.05, is shown in bold. The MLCBR at T4 was MTG_R for all ages and SD distances.

The brain regions correlated with the two orientational SD pairs at all 10-10 fiducial points and their normalized PPL from 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants are provided in Tables S3-7 in the Supplementary Material for SD distances of 10, 15, 20, 25, and 30 mm, respectively.

3.2.

Evaluation of SCC at All Fiducial Points

The SCC metrics NCBR, selectivity, and sensitivity at all 10-10 fiducial points are shown in Figs. 5Fig. 67, respectively. The summary statistics of the SCC metrics across all fiducial points are shown in Fig. 8.

Fig. 5

NCBR for all 10-10 fiducial points at five different SD distances for (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. Darker red regions indicate larger NCBR.

NPH_8_2_025009_f005.png

Fig. 6

Selectivity of MLCBR for all 10-10 fiducial points at five different SD distances for (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. Areas in magenta and blue indicate higher and lower selectivity, respectively.

NPH_8_2_025009_f006.png

Fig. 7

Sensitivity of MLCBR for all 10-10 fiducial points at five different SD distances for (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. Area in magenta and blue indicate higher and lower sensitivity, respectively.

NPH_8_2_025009_f007.png

Fig. 8

Box plots summarizing the SCC metrics, (a) NCBR; (b) selectivity; and (c) sensitivity, at all scalp fiducial points of the 10-10 system for each age and SD distance. The individual colored dots indicate the SCC metrics of each fiducial point. Boxes indicate the interquartile range. The black horizontal line within the boxes indicates the median. Whiskers extend 1.5 times above and below the interquartile range limits. Statistical significance of post-hoc test for (d) NCBR; (e) selectivity; and (f) sensitivity are indicated with matrices. * p<0.05, ** p<0.01, *** p<0.001, white and gray blanks are not significant and not applicable, respectively.

NPH_8_2_025009_f008.png

We calculated the NCBR value for all 10-10 fiducial points over five SD distances at three ages. As shown in Fig. 5 for circumferential SD pairs, fiducial points neighboring the longitudinal fissure, for example, Fpz, FCz, Cz, CPz, and POz, were correlated with more brain regions for every SD distance and age. Additionally, the larger the SD distance, the larger the NCBR of the fiducial points for every age group. The NCBR of 0-yo was larger than that of 1- and 2-yo, while the NCBR of 1- and 2-yo was similar for each SD distance. According to a nonparametric statistical analysis, we found significant main effects of SD distance [WTS(4)=195.88, p<0.001] and age [WTS(2)=10.37, p<0.01], but no interaction [WTS(8)=9.01, p=0.34]. For the age factor [Figs. 8(a) and 8(d) upper panel], multiple comparisons with the Bonferroni adjustment showed that the NCBR of 0-yo was larger than that of 1- and 2-yo (p<0.001, corrected); however, no significant differences were found between 1- and 2-yo (p=0.92, corrected). For the SD distance factor [Figs. 8(a) and 8(d) lower panel], multiple comparisons between NCBR of any two SD distances produced significantly different values, and the NCBR values of larger SD distances were greater than those of smaller SD distances (p<0.001, corrected). Similar results for the NCBR of the vertical SD pairs are shown in Fig. S2 in the Supplementary Material.

The selectivity of the MLCBR shown in Fig. 6 shows the ratio of the PPL in the MLCBR to its sum in the 90 AAL brain region. In a sense, the selectivity represents the probability that the fNIRS signal originates from the MLCBR. For most fiducial points, higher selectivity was observed for shorter SD distances, regardless of age. Moreover, the selectivity of 1- and 2-yo was larger than that of 0-yo. These observable findings were supported by statistically significant main effects of age [F(2,180)=4.89, p<0.05, ηG2=0.049] and SD distance [F(1.41,253.25)=266.11, p<0.001, ηG2=0.067] on selectivity; however, no statistically significant two-way interactions were found between age and SD distance on the selectivity, F(2.81,253.25)=0.93, p=0.42, ηG2=0.001 [Fig. 8(b)]. The results of the post-hoc multiple comparisons are shown in Fig. 8(e). For the age factor [Fig. 8(e), upper panel], multiple pairwise independent sample t-tests showed that the selectivity of 0-yo was lower than that of 1- and 2-yo for all fiducial points on average (p<0.001, Bonferroni-corrected). In contrast, multiple pairwise paired t-tests for the SD distance [Fig. 8(e), lower panel] showed that comparisons from any two SD distances were significantly different (p<0.001, corrected) and that the selectivity decreased with increasing SD distance. Similar results for the selectivity of vertical SD pairs are shown in Fig. S3 in the Supplementary Material.

With regard to the sensitivity of the MLCBR over all 10-10 fiducial points, we found an obvious increase in Labs,MLCBR with increasing SD distance for each age (Fig. 7). However, no differences in age were observed during the visual inspection. The 3 (age) × 5 (SD distance) mixed ANOVA revealed significant main effects of age [F(2,180)=4.87, p<0.01, ηG2=0.047] and SD distance [F(1.37,247.25)=837.02, p<0.001, ηG2=0.306]. However, these main effects were further qualified by the presence of a significant interaction between age and SD distance, F(2.75,247.25)=8.67, p<0.001, ηG2=0.009 [Fig. 8(c)]. The simple main effect of age was significant for all SD distances, that is, 10, 15, 20, 25, and 30 mm (all p<0.05). The results of the post-hoc multiple comparisons are shown in Fig. 8(f). At a 10-mm SD distance, the mean sensitivity of 1-yo was significantly higher than that of 2-yo (p<0.05, Bonferroni-corrected). The mean sensitivity of 1-yo was significantly higher than that of 0-yo at SD distances of 15 mm (p<0.05, corrected), 20 mm (p<0.01, corrected), 25 mm (p<0.01, corrected), and 30 mm (p<0.01, corrected). The simple main effect of the SD distance was also significant for any of the 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants (all p<0.001). At all years of age, the sensitivity of larger SD distances was significantly greater than that of smaller SD distances [Figs. 8(c) and 8(f), p<0.001, corrected]. Similar results for the sensitivity of the vertical SD pairs are presented in Fig. S4 in the Supplementary Material.

We found that almost half of 10-10 fiducial points correlated with the completely consistent MLCBR across the three ages for each SD distance (Fig. 9). The average number of fiducial points correlated with the completely consistent MLCBR for all SD distances was 41.6±1.5 and 39.6±0.9 for circumferential and vertical SD pairs, respectively, as shown in green circles in Fig. 9. However, fiducial points correlated with inconsistent MLCBR across the three ages (yellow and red circles) were found around the longitudinal fissure for both SD pair orientations. To help fNIRS researchers examine longitudinal functional development from 0- to 2-yo, we have also provided fiducial points whose MLCBR was completely consistent at certain SD distances for the circumferential and vertical SD pairs across 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants (Table 4).

Fig. 9

Consistency of MLCBR at five SD distances for (a) circumferential and (b) vertical SD pairs. The consistency of the MLCBR among 0-yo, 1-yo, and 2-yo for every fiducial point is indicated by circles with different colors.

NPH_8_2_025009_f009.png

Table 4

Summary of fiducial points whose MLCBR was completely consistent at some SD distances for the circumferential and vertical SD pairs across 0-yo, 1-yo, and 2-yo infants.

Fiducial pointCircumferential SD pairsVertical SD pairs
MLCBRSD distance (mm)MLCBRSD distance (mm)
CzSMA-R15
AFzSFGmed-R20, 25, 30
FzSFGmed-R20
CPzPoCG-L20, 25, 30
POzSPG-L20, 25, 30
T3MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
C5STG-L25, 30PoCG-L25, 30
C3PoCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30PoCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
C1PreCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30PreCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
C2PreCG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30PreCG-R10, 15, 30
C4PoCG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30PoCG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
C6PoCG-R10, 20, 25, 30
T4MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
FT7TPOsup-L10, 15, 20, 25TPOsup-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F7ORBinf-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30ORBinf-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
AF7ORBmid-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30ORBmid-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
Fp1ORBmid-L20, 25, 30
Fp2SFGdor-R25, 30
AF8ORBmid-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30ORBmid-R10, 15
F8ORBinf-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30ORBinf-R10, 15, 20
FT8MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-R10, 20, 25, 30
TP7MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
T5MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
O1MOG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MOG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
O2SOG-R10, 15, 20SOG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
PO8MOG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MOG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
T6MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
TP8MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MTG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
FC5IFGtriang-L20, 25, 30IFGoperc-L10, 20, 30
FC3MFG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
FC1MFG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-L15, 20, 25, 30
FC2MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
FC4MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
FC6IFGoperc-R10, 30IFGoperc-R10
F5IFGtriang-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30IFGtriang-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F3MFG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F1SFGdor-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F2SFGdor-R10, 15SFGdor-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F4MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30MFG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
F6IFGtriang-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30IFGtriang-R10, 15, 20, 25
AF3MFG-L25, 30MFG-L20, 25, 30
AF4MFG-R30
CP3IPL-L20
CP1PoCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30PoCG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
CP2PoCG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30PoCG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
CP4IPL-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30IPL-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
CP6SMG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30SMG-R10, 15, 20, 25
P5ANG-L10, 15, 20, 25ANG-L10, 15, 20
P3ANG-L10, 15ANG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
P1SPG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30SPG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
P2SPG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30SPG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
P4ANG-R10, 15, 20, 25ANG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
P6ANG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30ANG-R10, 15, 20, 25, 30
PO3ANG-L10, 15, 20, 25, 30
Note: the line ‘–’ indicates that the category is not applicable. See Table S2 in the Supplementary Material for abbreviations of brain regions.

3.3.

Suitable SD Distances for infant fNIRS Based on SCC Metrics

Based on the results in Sec. 3.2, we found that the sensitivity and selectivity of the MLCBR increased and decreased, respectively, as the SD distance increased. In other words, there is a trade-off between the sensitivity and selectivity of the MLCBR in determining a suitable SD distance for targeting the brain regions of interest in infant fNIRS. Therefore, a suitable SD distance that achieves a good balance between the sensitivity and selectivity of the MLCBR in infant fNIRS was explored.

First, we drew a scatter plot of the selectivity and sensitivity for all 10-10 fiducial points at five SD distances at each infant age [Figs. 10(a)10(c)]. Then, we empirically chose thresholds 3 and 0.4 for the sensitivity and the selectivity, respectively, to categorize every point in the scatter plot into three zones. Because sensitivity takes precedence over selectivity in fNIRS, we defined the red zone as an area with a sensitivity <3. The remaining region was divided into two zones based on the selectivity threshold. Fiducial points with a sensitivity lower and >0.4, were classified into yellow and green zones, respectively. Therefore, the SD distance with a larger number of fiducial points categorized into the green zone indicates that both higher sensitivity and selectivity can be achieved.

Fig. 10

Relation between sensitivity and selectivity for each SD distance in (a) 0-yo; (b) 1-yo; and (c) 2-yo. Black circles in figures (a)–(c) indicate data from each 10-10 fiducial point. (d) Ratios of the number of fiducial points in the green, yellow, and red zones to the number of all 10-10 fiducial points.

NPH_8_2_025009_f010.png

As shown in Fig. 10, the distribution of points in the scatter plot varies with the SD distance for infants at all ages [Figs. 10(a)10(c) for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants, respectively]. For shorter and longer SD distances, more fiducial points were observed in the red and yellow zones, respectively. The ratios of the number of fiducial points in each zone to all fiducial points are shown in Fig. 10(d). For the green zone, inverted U-shaped curves were observed for all three age groups. On the other hand, the ratio of fiducial points in the yellow and red zones showed a monotonic increase and decrease as the SD distance increased. These findings suggest that excessive short and long SD distances are unsuitable for infant fNIRS. Therefore, the SD distance between 15 mm and 25 mm could be suitable for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants. A 25 mm SD distance was more suitable if the sensitivity was emphasized, whereas a 15-mm SD distance was more appropriate if the selectivity was considered as a priority. A relatively balanced trade-off can be obtained when the SD distance is 20 mm. To validate the robustness of this finding, we examined the same problem with different thresholds 4 and 0.5 for sensitivity and selectivity, respectively. Although the thresholds changed modestly, the shapes of the graph, as shown in Fig. 10(d), were maintained (Fig. S5 in the Supplementary Material).

4.

Discussion

fNIRS has contributed significantly to the advancement of developmental cognitive neuroscience; however, fNIRS data cannot provide any anatomical brain information, which is critical for data explanation and comparisons with other modalities. Although several methods have been proposed to obtain SCC in adults and infants, little is known about the influence of age and SD distance on SCC with substantial physical development of the infant’s head during the first two postnatal years. In the present study, we adopted light propagation analysis to establish a precise optics-based SCC between the fNIRS measurement channels, that is, SD pairs, set on the 10-10 system scalp positions and AAL brain regions in three age-appropriate head models of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo. Importantly, we provided four metrics: NCBR, and the selectivity, sensitivity, and consistency of the MLCBR, to quantitatively evaluate the SCC for changes during a remarkable period of brain development. Moreover, we assessed the suitable SD distances for infant fNIRS by simultaneously considering the selectivity and sensitivity of the MLCBR.

4.1.

Optics-Based SCC Derived from Age-Appropriate Sophisticated Head Models

Several studies have taken an important step toward the establishment of infant SCC;2225 however, they only considered the head size of infants based on the simplified fNIRS principle that the signal comes from the cortical projection point below the midpoint of the SD pair. In fact, an fNIRS channel measures absorption changes in a broad cortical area, rather than at a single point. Thus, to establish a precise optics-based SCC that reflects light diffusion in the brains of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants, the key step is to construct sophisticated realistic head models that comprise multiple biological tissues with distinct optical properties such as scalp, skull, CSF, and brain tissues.

In this study, we constructed a five-layered head model for each age, distinguishable from a four-layered model where the skull and scalp constitute a single extracerebral layer.36,46,59 Separating the scalp and skull in infantile light propagation analysis could guarantee more accurate SCC compared with using the four-layered model, as these two types of infant tissues have their own optical properties35,51 and physical development.43,44

In addition, we employed template-based head models for light propagation analyses, because the acquisition of subject-specific MRI is generally challenging owing to the difficulty in controlling the motion of infants and undermines the intrinsic advantage of the fNIRS technique for facilitating functional brain measurements during early development. Notably, previous adult or infant studies have shown that the use of a template-based head model is useful for identifying the activation focus, although there are anatomical differences between subject-specific and template-based head models.24,25,6062 Furthermore, the template-based model used in this study was derived from anatomical images of 95 healthy infants and reflects the general anatomical structure of infants. Therefore, the optics-based SCC obtained by analyzing light propagation in template-based infant head models of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo could provide a reliable database to guide fNIRS users in designing probe geometry, and to explain fNIRS data.

Light propagation analysis depends on the optical properties of head tissues. In this study, the same optical properties of the neonate were assigned to all three age models because no established optical properties of head tissues were available for each age group in this study. On the other hand, optical properties in these ages may change due to alterations in tissue composition, such as myelination and bone mineral density during the first 2 years of life. As the level of bone mineral density was almost constant during the first year63 and slightly increased in the second year,64 the optical properties of the skull may not change during those years. In contrast, a substantial amount of myelination occurs in a wide range of brains.65 Thus, the alteration of the optical properties of WM and GM will definitely be at a non-negligible level. Light propagation is affected by differences in optical properties between adults and infants.35 To address the effect of changes in optical properties on the SCC, we attempted to use the optical properties of the GM and WM of adults to calculate the SCC for the oldest 2-yo head model. Compared to the results with infants’ optical properties, additional simulation results revealed that the optical properties of adults applied to 2-yo had little effect on the SCC. Although a slightly broadened SSP was observed owing to the stronger scattering (Fig. S6 in the Supplementary Material for comparison between the optical properties of the neonate and adult), the consistency of the MLCBR among 0-, 1-, and 2-yo was unaffected by the difference in the optical properties (see Fig. S7 and Table S8 in the Supplementary Material for the optical properties of the adults). The actual changes in optical properties due to development from 0- to 2-yo would be smaller than those assumed here for neonates to adults. Therefore, we claim that our findings using the same optical properties for all three age groups could be used as a reference for developmental cognitive neuroscience. Meanwhile, the accuracy of optics-based SCC will be improved with the advance of in vivo measurement techniques for characterizing the optical properties of tissues.66 However, the measurement of accurate optical properties in vivo is still a challenging issue, as shown by the wide range of values reported by measurement techniques.36

4.2.

Effect of SD Distance and Physical Development on the SCC

For a given fiducial point, the SSP was found to be a function of the SD distance, that is, the spatial range of the SSP broadened as the SD distance increased. This finding was consistent with previous studies, regardless of slab models30,67 or realistic head models68 being utilized. Intriguingly, the spatial distribution of the SSP values was also affected by age for a consistent SD distance (Fig. 4). Age-related alterations in SSP may be caused by changes in the local anatomical structures across the three age groups.

The optics-based SCC results (Table 4 and Table. S3-7 in the Supplementary Material) demonstrate that fNIRS signals interrogated by an SD pair always originate from multiple brain regions, even if the SD distance is 10 mm. The one-to-many relationship between an SD pair and cortical regions is due to strong light scattering in the head tissue. In addition, we found several aspects related to NCBR. As for the spatial characteristics, fiducial points around the longitudinal fissure were correlated with more brain regions than other points (Fig. 5), which may be because the cortices around the longitudinal fissure were parcellated into a relatively large number of regions in the AAL atlas. From the perspective of age-related change, we found that although the NCBR of 0-yo was larger than that of 1- and 2-yo, there were no significant differences between the NCBR values of 1-o and 2-yo [Fig. 8(d)], which suggests that the similar spatial range of SSP for the same SD distance will occupy fewer brain regions when the head becomes larger owing to the physical development of the infant. In terms of the dependency on the SD distance, the finding that a larger SD distance had more corresponding brain regions [Figs. 5, 8(a), and 8(d)] could be clearly explained by the wider SSP at larger SD distances, as shown in Fig. 4.

We found that the selectivity of MLCBR decreased with increasing SD distance [Figs. 6 and 8(b)]. As mentioned before, the larger the SD distance, the wider the spatial range of the SSP in the brain. Hence, the increase in the normalized PPL of brain regions other than the MLCBR resulted in a relative decrease in the normalized PPL of the MLCBR. In addition, the selectivity of 1- and 2-yo was higher than that of 0-yo on average for all fiducial points (Fig. 6). The difference in the physical development of the head in each year would have determined the degree of age-dependent change in the normalized PPL of the MLCBR. The 0-yo head size is obviously smaller than that of 1- and 2-yo, while 1- and 2-yo have an almost similar head size (see Fig. 1 for example). In other words, the constant area of the brain surface is occupied by a small number of brain regions in the large brains of 1- and 2-yo, but by a large number in the small brains of 0-yo. Furthermore, the spatial range of the SSP was almost constant for the same SD distance, regardless of age. Thus, the constant spatial range of SSP across age was mainly occupied by the MLCBR in the larger brains of 1- and 2-yo, but distributed across several brain regions in the smaller brain of the 0-yo. Therefore, the normalized PPL in the MLCBR of the 0-yo infants was significantly smaller than that of the 1- and 2-yo infants.

We found that the sensitivity of the MLCBR significantly increased as the SD distance increased from 10 to 30 mm at 5-mm intervals for each age [Fig. 8(f)]. This finding is in accordance with evidence obtained from the change in GM sensitivity as a function of SD distance in the neonatal and adult head models.35 The sensitivity of 1-yo was significantly higher than that of 0-yo at 15-, 20-, 25-, and 30-mm SD distances; however, the sensitivity of 2-yo was significantly lower than that of 1-yo at a 10 mm SD distance [Fig. 8(f)]. One possible explanation for the increase or decrease in the sensitivity depending on the time of growth is that physical development differs between brain tissue and superficial layers such as the scalp and skull; the increase in the sensitivity from 0- to 1-yo may be caused by the considerable expansion of the cortical surface during this period. Conversely, from 1- to 2-yo, the decrease in sensitivity may be due to relatively smaller expansion of the cortical surface, although the thickness of the superficial layers continues to increase.

We also found that the MLCBR was completely consistent at almost half of 10-10 fiducial points of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo for each SD distance. In other words, almost half of the fiducial points were correlated with one specific brain region with the largest normalized PPL (Fig. 9 and Table 4). This finding indicates that we can measure identical MLCBRs in longitudinal studies from 0- to 2-yo with fNIRS without the subject’s own structural MRI when a probe of the same SD distance is attached at the scalp fiducial points. Thus, this finding is extremely uplifting for fNIRS users who are interested in the functional development of such MLCBRs. On the other hand, the MLCBR corresponding to fiducial points just above the longitudinal fissure was mostly inconsistent across the three ages (Fig. 9). The NCBR and Lnorm,MLCBR of the fiducial points were relatively larger and smaller than those of the other points, respectively (Figs. 5 and 6), which suggests that the MLCBR at a certain age may easily be replaced by another brain region at another age because of the slight change in the light path. One possibility for age-related changes in the light path is that the thickness of the skull in the frontal, parietal, and occipital regions along with the longitudinal fissure changes more drastically than in other locations from 0- to 2-yo.44 Another possibility is that the cortical surface expands relatively more in regions of the superior parietal, prefrontal, occipital cortices, and postcentral gyrus than in other regions during the first two years.2

The consistency of the MLCBR revealed in the present study could provide strong support for the reliability of findings from longitudinal developmental studies using fNIRS in which the probe was attached based on scalp fiducial points. In recent years, by executing the longitudinal cohort study projects [e.g., Brain Imaging for Global Health (BRIGHT) project], several laboratories worldwide have devoted themselves to investigating functional brain development during the first two postnatal years, including studies of visual working memory,69 social cognition,70,71 and resting-state functional networks.72 The present study will be especially useful for a large cohort of longitudinal or cross-sectional studies of early brain development, as structural MRI scanning, which is a demanding task for young populations, is not necessarily required.

4.3.

Consideration of Suitable SD Distances for Infant fNIRS

The suitable SD distance for infants remains a matter of debate, as its choice is dependent on several factors. By considering a trade-off between the selectivity and sensitivity of the MLCBR, this study provided recommendations on suitable SD distances for 0-, 1-, and 2-yo. The most desirable SD distance is the one that has both the maximum number of fiducial points with high selectivity and sensitivity, and a minimum number of fiducial points with low selectivity and sensitivity. Based on this perspective, we determined that the suitable SD distances in fNIRS for infants during the first 2 years ranged from 15 to 25 mm. This finding supports the results of a previous study in which the highest sensitivity was obtained at an SD distance of 20 mm in 3-month-old infants.39 In practical applications, we suggest that fNIRS users choose the SD distance according to the criteria (sensitivity or selectivity) more important to them. If the users focus on selectivity, an SD distance of 15 mm could be a better choice, whereas 25 mm is suitable for sensitivity as the priority. In addition, if fNIRS users pursue a relatively balanced trade-off between selectivity and sensitivity, we recommend an SD distance of 20 mm. Our findings also revealed that the optimal SD distance for infants is different from that for adults. Previous adult studies have demonstrated that fNIRS probes should ideally be designed with 30- to 35-mm SD distances,68,73 as most light passes through the extra-cerebral superficial tissues at SD distances <20  mm.67 Compared with that for adults, and the most suitable SD distances for infants are smaller than 30 mm, which indicates that thin superficial layers allow much more photons to pass through the brain tissue.

4.4.

Future Directions and Limitations

In the current study, we found that the orientation of SD pairs over 10-10 positions had no significant effect on our results, which was consistent with findings from photon propagation in the adult brain.34,38 Notably, only the AAL atlas was adopted to obtain the SCC in this study. The orientation of SD pairs may influence the SCC when other infant brain atlases with finer anatomical parcellation are available with advances in MRI techniques.

For the application of this work, the precise optics-based SCC for SD pairs at all 10-10 system scalp locations (Table S3-7 in the Supplementary Material) can be used as a reference table to guide fNIRS users in designing the probe arrangement for targeting specific brain regions, as well as to help them explain fNIRS data obtained according to the 10-10 system. Importantly, the summary of scalp positions whose MLCBR was completely consistent across 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants (Table 4) is beneficial for the investigation of functional development for one specific brain region. However, the spatial density of the 10-10 fiducial points for analyzing the optics-based SCC would be relatively sparse because a few brain regions had only a small chance of being corresponded. Our future work will calculate the optics-based SCC by using a denser placement system,74 which can guarantee all brain regions with a high probability of being corresponded and estimate underlying brain regions for any arrangement of SD pairs. In addition, the established age-appropriate infant head models and the proposed method for calculating the optics-based SCC can be used to estimate brain regions at any scalp position by virtually placing the SD pairs on the scalp of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo infants, even without infant head structures.

The head tissues within the first postnatal year undergo dramatic growth, such as the total volume of the brain expanding up to double its size.44,75,76 Furthermore, several studies have reported the discovery of longitudinal functional brain development during this period.71,77,78 However, the 0-yo infant head model used in this study was constructed with a template-based MRI of an approximately 1-month-old infant. Establishing optics-based SCC at a fine temporal scale (every 3 months from birth to 12 months), which is in line with physical development during the first year after birth, it is expected to provide useful information for investigating the longitudinal development of brain functions using the fNIRS technique.

5.

Conclusion

The present study adopted light propagation analysis to establish a precise optics-based SCC between fNIRS measurement channels (i.e., SD pairs) set on the 10-10 system scalp positions and AAL brain regions in three age-appropriate head models of 0-, 1-, and 2-yo. We found that age-related changes in the SCC metrics, particularly the fiducial points around the longitudinal fissure, correlated with different brain regions across the three age groups. In contrast, the MLCBR was consistent across the three ages for more than half of the fiducial points. Furthermore, we recommend SD distances between 15 and 25 mm as suitable for fNIRS in infants by simultaneously considering the selectivity and sensitivity of the MLCBR. We sincerely hope that age-appropriate SCC will be a useful reference to guide the design of fNIRS probes and provide convincing anatomical interpretations of fNIRS data for future infant developmental studies.

Disclosures

The authors have declared that no conflict of interest exists.

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank our anonymous reviewers for their assistance in clarifying and enhancing this work.

Code, Data, and Materials Availability

Data supporting the results reported in the paper can be requested by contacting the corresponding author.

References

1. 

J. H. Gilmore et al., “Longitudinal development of cortical and subcortical gray matter from birth to 2 years,” Cereb. Cortex, 22 (11), 2478 –2485 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhr327 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

2. 

G. Li et al., “Mapping region-specific longitudinal cortical surface expansion from birth to 2 years of age,” Cereb. Cortex, 23 (11), 2724 –2733 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs265 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

3. 

G. Li et al., “Mapping longitudinal development of local cortical gyrification in infants from birth to 2 years of age,” J. Neurosci., 34 (12), 4228 –4238 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3976-13.2014 Google Scholar

4. 

J. X. Nie et al., “Longitudinal development of cortical thickness, folding, and fiber density networks in the first 2 years of life,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 35 (8), 3726 –3737 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22432 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

5. 

F. Wang et al., “Developmental topography of cortical thickness during infancy,” Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA, 116 (32), 15855 –15860 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1821523116 Google Scholar

6. 

J. Kagan and N. Herschkowitz, A Young Mind in a Growing Brain, Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Mahwah, NJ (2005). Google Scholar

7. 

J. B. Girault et al., “White matter microstructural development and cognitive ability in the first 2 years of life,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 40 (4), 1195 –1210 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24439 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

8. 

S. Lloyd-Fox, A. Blasi and C. E. Elwell, “Illuminating the developing brain: the past, present and future of functional near infrared spectroscopy,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 34 (3), 269 –284 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.008 NBREDE 0149-7634 Google Scholar

9. 

R. E. Vanderwert and C. A. Nelson, “The use of near-infrared spectroscopy in the study of typical and atypical development,” Neuroimage, 85 264 –271 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.009 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

10. 

R. N. Aslin, M. Shukla and L. L. Emberson, “Hemodynamic correlates of cognition in human infants,” Annu. Rev. Psychol., 66 349 –379 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115108 ARPSAC 0066-4308 Google Scholar

11. 

N. M. McDonald and K. L. Perdue, “The infant brain in the social world: moving toward interactive social neuroscience with functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev., 87 38 –49 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.007 NBREDE 0149-7634 Google Scholar

12. 

D. A. Boas et al., “Twenty years of functional near-infrared spectroscopy: introduction for the special issue,” Neuroimage, 85 1 –5 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.033 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

13. 

F. Scholkmann et al., “A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology,” Neuroimage, 85 6 –27 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

14. 

V. Quaresima and M. Ferrari, “A mini-review on functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS): Where do we stand, and where should we go?,” Photonics-Basel, 6 (3), 87 (2019). https://doi.org/10.3390/photonics6030087 Google Scholar

15. 

P. Pinti et al., “The present and future use of functional near‐infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) for cognitive neuroscience,” Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 1464 (1), 5 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1111/nyas.13948 ANYAA9 0077-8923 Google Scholar

16. 

V. Jurcak, D. Tsuzuki and I. Dan, “10/20, 10/10, and 10/5 systems revisited: their validity as relative head-surface-based positioning systems,” Neuroimage, 34 (4), 1600 –1611 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.024 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

17. 

Y. Minagawa-Kawai et al., “Neural attunement processes in infants during the acquisition of a language-specific phonemic contrast,” J. Neurosci., 27 (2), 315 –321 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1984-06.2007 JNRSDS 0270-6474 Google Scholar

18. 

G. Taga and K. Asakawa, “Selectivity and localization of cortical response to auditory and visual stimulation in awake infants aged 2 to 4 months,” Neuroimage, 36 (4), 1246 –1252 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.04.037 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

19. 

S. Lloyd-Fox et al., “Social perception in infancy: a near infrared spectroscopy study,” Child Dev., 80 (4), 986 –999 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01312.x CHDEAW 0009-3920 Google Scholar

20. 

M. Okamoto et al., “Three-dimensional probabilistic anatomical cranio-cerebral correlation via the international 10-20 system oriented for transcranial functional brain mapping,” Neuroimage, 21 (1), 99 –111 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.08.026 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

21. 

L. Koessler et al., “Automated cortical projection of EEG sensors: anatomical correlation via the international 10-10 system,” Neuroimage, 46 (1), 64 –72 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.02.006 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

22. 

C. Kabdebon et al., “Anatomical correlations of the international 10-20 sensor placement system in infants,” Neuroimage, 99 342 –356 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.046 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

23. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Macroanatomical landmarks featuring junctions of major sulci and fissures and scalp landmarks based on the international 10-10 system for analyzing lateral cortical development of infants,” Front. Neurosci., 11 394 (2017). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00394 1662-453X Google Scholar

24. 

S. Lloyd-Fox et al., “Coregistering functional near-infrared spectroscopy with underlying cortical areas in infants,” Neurophotonics, 1 (2), 025006 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.1.2.025006 Google Scholar

25. 

L. L. Emberson et al., “Using fNIRS to examine occipital and temporal responses to stimulus repetition in young infants: Evidence of selective frontal cortex involvement,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci., 23 26 –38 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.002 Google Scholar

26. 

D. Tsuzuki et al., “Virtual spatial registration of stand-alone MRS data to MNI space,” Neuroimage, 34 (4), 1506 –1518 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.10.043 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

27. 

H. Watanabe et al., “Effect of auditory input on activations in infant diverse cortical regions during audiovisual processing,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 34 (3), 543 –565 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21453 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

28. 

Y. Minagawa et al., “Infant word segmentation recruits the cerebral network of phonological short-term memory,” Brain Lang., 170 39 –49 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2017.03.005 Google Scholar

29. 

Y. Hakuno et al., “Interactive live fNIRS reveals engagement of the temporoparietal junction in response to social contingency in infants,” Neuroimage, 218 116901 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116901 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

30. 

E. Okada et al., “Theoretical and experimental investigation of near-infrared light propagation in a model of the adult head,” Appl. Opt., 36 (1), 21 –31 (1997). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.36.000021 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

31. 

M. Firbank, E. Okada and D. T. Delpy, “A theoretical study of the signal contribution of regions of the adult head to near-infrared spectroscopy studies of visual evoked responses,” Neuroimage, 8 (1), 69 –78 (1998). https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.1998.0348 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

32. 

Y. Hoshi et al., “Reevaluation of near-infrared light propagation in the adult human head: implications for functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” J. Biomed. Opt., 10 (6), 064032 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.2142325 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

33. 

T. Koyama et al., “Practical and adequate approach to modeling light propagation in an adult head with low-scattering regions by use of diffusion theory,” Appl. Opt., 44 (11), 2094 –2103 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.44.002094 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

34. 

G. E. Strangman, Q. Zhang and Z. Li, “Scalp and skull influence on near infrared photon propagation in the Colin27 brain template,” Neuroimage, 85 136 –149 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.04.090 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

35. 

Y. Fukui, Y. Ajichi and E. Okada, “Monte Carlo prediction of near-infrared light propagation in realistic adult and neonatal head models,” Appl. Opt., 42 (16), 2881 –2887 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.42.002881 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

36. 

M. Dehaes et al., “Assessment of the frequency-domain multi-distance method to evaluate the brain optical properties: Monte Carlo simulations from neonate to adult,” Biomed. Opt. Express, 2 (3), 552 –567 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1364/BOE.2.000552 BOEICL 2156-7085 Google Scholar

37. 

G. A. Z. Morais, J. B. Balardin and J. R. Sato, “fNIRS optodes’ location decider (fOLD): a toolbox for probe arrangement guided by brain regions-of-interest,” Sci Rep, 8 3341 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-21716-z Google Scholar

38. 

L. Cai et al., “Targeting brain regions of interest for functional near-infrared spectroscopy—scalp-cortex correlation using subject-specific light propagation models,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 42 1969 –1986 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.25367 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

39. 

G. Taga, F. Homae and H. Watanabe, “Effects of source-detector distance of near infrared spectroscopy on the measurement of the cortical hemodynamic response in infants,” Neuroimage, 38 (3), 452 –460 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.050 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

40. 

F. Shi et al., “Infant brain atlases from neonates to 1-and 2-year-olds,” PLoS One, 6 (4), e18746 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0018746 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

41. 

N. Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., “Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain,” Neuroimage, 15 (1), 273 –289 (2002). https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0978 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

42. 

UNC IDEA Lab, “UNC 0-1-2 Infant Atlases,” (2020) https://www.nitrc.org/projects/pediatricatlas/ Google Scholar

43. 

R. W. Young, “Age changes in the thickness of the scalp in white males,” Hum. Biol., 31 (1), 74 –79 (1959). HUBIAA 0018-7143 Google Scholar

44. 

Z. G. Li et al., “A statistical skull geometry model for children 0–3 years old,” PLoS One, 10 (5), e0127322 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0127322 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

45. 

Q. Q. Fang and D. A. Boas, “Tetrahedral mesh generation from volumetric binary and grayscale images,” in IEEE Int. Symp. Biomed. Imaging: From Nano to Macro, 1142 –1145 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1109/ISBI.2009.5193259 Google Scholar

46. 

S. Brigadoi et al., “A 4D neonatal head model for diffuse optical imaging of pre-term to term infants,” Neuroimage, 100 385 –394 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.06.028 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

47. 

A. Liu and B. Joe, “Relationship between tetrahedron shape measures,” BIT Numer. Math., 34 (2), 268 –287 (1994). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01955874 BITTEL 0006-3835 Google Scholar

48. 

Optmed Lab, Dartmouth Colledge, “NIRFAST open source software for multi-modal optical molecular imaging,” (2018) http://www.dartmouth.edu/~nir/nirfast/ Google Scholar

49. 

H. Dehghani et al., “Near infrared optical tomography using NIRFAST: algorithm for numerical model and image reconstruction,” Commun. Numer. Methods Eng., 25 (6), 711 –732 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1002/cnm.1162 CANMER 0748-8025 Google Scholar

50. 

M. Jermyn et al., “Fast segmentation and high-quality three-dimensional volume mesh creation from medical images for diffuse optical tomography,” J. Biomed. Opt., 18 (8), 086007 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.JBO.18.8.086007 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

51. 

M. Dehaes et al., “Quantitative effect of the neonatal fontanel on synthetic near infrared spectroscopy measurements,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 34 (4), 878 –889 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21483 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

52. 

M. R. Nuwer, “10-10 electrode system for EEG recording,” Clin. Neurophysiol., 129 (5), 1103 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.clinph.2018.01.065 CNEUFU 1388-2457 Google Scholar

53. 

Y. Oki, H. Kawaguchi and E. Okada, “Validation of practical diffusion approximation for virtual near infrared spectroscopy using a digital head phantom,” Opt. Rev., 16 (2), 153 –159 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10043-009-0026-3 1340-6000 Google Scholar

54. 

S. R. Arridge, “Photon-measurement density functions. Part I: analytical forms,” Appl. Opt., 34 (31), 7395 –7409 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.34.007395 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

55. 

S. R. Arridge and M. Schweiger, “Photon-measurement density functions. Part 2: finite-element-method calculations,” Appl. Opt., 34 (34), 8026 –8037 (1995). https://doi.org/10.1364/AO.34.008026 APOPAI 0003-6935 Google Scholar

56. 

K. Noguchi et al., “nparLD: an R software package for the nonparametric analysis of longitudinal data in factorial experiments,” J. Stat. Software, 50 (12), 1 –23 (2012). https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v050.i12 Google Scholar

57. 

S. Olejnik and J. Algina, “Generalized eta and omega squared statistics: measures of effect size for some common research designs,” Psychol. Methods, 8 (4), 434 –447 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.8.4.434 Google Scholar

58. 

R. Bakeman, “Recommended effect size statistics for repeated measures designs,” Behav. Res. Methods, 37 (3), 379 –384 (2005). https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192707 Google Scholar

59. 

S. L. Ferradal et al., “Functional imaging of the developing brain at the bedside using diffuse optical tomography,” Cereb. Cortex, 26 (4), 1558 –1568 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu320 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

60. 

A. Custo et al., “Anatomical atlas-guided diffuse optical tomography of brain activation,” Neuroimage, 49 (1), 561 –567 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.07.033 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

61. 

R. J. Cooper et al., “Validating atlas-guided DOT: a comparison of diffuse optical tomography informed by atlas and subject-specific anatomies,” Neuroimage, 62 (3), 1999 –2006 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.05.031 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

62. 

S. L. Ferradal et al., “Atlas-based head modeling and spatial normalization for high-density diffuse optical tomography: in vivo validation against fMRI,” Neuroimage, 85 117 –126 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.069 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

63. 

S. Gallo, C. A. Vanstone and H. A. Weiler, “Normative data for bone mass in healthy term infants from birth to 1 year of age,” J Osteoporos, 2012 1 –8 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1155/2012/672403 Google Scholar

64. 

H. Delye et al., “Creating a normative database of age-specific 3D geometrical data, bone density, and bone thickness of the developing skull: a pilot study,” J. Neurosurg. Pediatr., 16 (6), 687 –702 (2015). https://doi.org/10.3171/2015.4.PEDS1493 Google Scholar

65. 

R. B. Dietrich et al., “MR evaluation of early myelination patterns in normal and developmentally delayed infants,” AJR Am. J. Roentgenol., 150 (4), 889 –896 (1988). https://doi.org/10.2214/ajr.150.4.889 Google Scholar

66. 

L. Spinelli et al., “In vivo measure of neonate brain optical properties and hemodynamic parameters by time-domain near-infrared spectroscopy,” Neurophotonics, 4 (4), 041414 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.4.4.041414 Google Scholar

67. 

L. Wang, H. Ayaz and M. Izzetoglu, “Investigation of the source‐detector separation in near infrared spectroscopy for healthy and clinical applications,” J. Biophotonics, 12 (11), e201900175 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1002/jbio.201900175 Google Scholar

68. 

G. E. Strangman, Z. Li and Q. Zhang, “Depth sensitivity and source-detector separations for near infrared spectroscopy based on the Colin27 brain template,” PLoS One, 8 (8), e66319 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0066319 POLNCL 1932-6203 Google Scholar

69. 

L. D. Reyes et al., “The functional brain networks that underlie visual working memory in the first two years of life,” Neuroimage, 219 116971 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116971 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

70. 

S. Lloyd-Fox et al., “Cortical specialisation to social stimuli from the first days to the second year of life: a rural Gambian cohort,” Dev. Cognit. Neurosci., 25 92 –104 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.005 Google Scholar

71. 

H. O. Miguel et al., “Infant brain response to affective and discriminative touch: a longitudinal study using fNIRS,” Social Neurosci., 14 (5), 571 –582 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2018.1536000 Google Scholar

72. 

C. Bulgarelli et al., “The developmental trajectory of fronto‐temporoparietal connectivity as a proxy of the default mode network: a longitudinal fNIRS investigation,” Hum. Brain Mapp., 41 (10), 2717 –2740 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.24974 HBRME7 1065-9471 Google Scholar

73. 

T. Li, H. Gong and Q. Luo, “Visualization of light propagation in visible Chinese human head for functional near-infrared spectroscopy,” J. Biomed. Opt., 16 (4), 045001 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.3567085 JBOPFO 1083-3668 Google Scholar

74. 

R. Oostenveld and P. Praamstra, “The five percent electrode system for high-resolution EEG and ERP measurements,” Clin. Neurophysiol., 112 (4), 713 –719 (2001). https://doi.org/10.1016/S1388-2457(00)00527-7 CNEUFU 1388-2457 Google Scholar

75. 

G. Li et al., “Computational neuroanatomy of baby brains: a review,” Neuroimage, 185 906 –925 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.042 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

76. 

W. Gao et al., “Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: plastic and modifiable,” Neuroscientist, 23 (2), 169 –184 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858416635986 NROSFJ 1073-8584 Google Scholar

77. 

W. Gao et al., “Functional network development during the first year: relative sequence and socioeconomic correlations,” Cereb. Cortex, 25 (9), 2919 –2928 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu088 53OPAV 1047-3211 Google Scholar

78. 

X. Y. Wen et al., “First-year development of modules and hubs in infant brain functional networks,” Neuroimage, 185 222 –235 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.10.019 NEIMEF 1053-8119 Google Scholar

Biography

Lin Cai is a PhD candidate in the Department of Electronics and Electrical Engineering, Keio University. His research interests mainly focus on the use of fNIRS to investigate cognitive development and the resting-state functional brain network during infancy and childhood. When working on his PhD, he has been investigating the brain mechanisms in early language acquisition at Keio Baby Lab and the modeling of light propagation in infants’ heads at Okada Lab.

Eiji Okada is a professor in the Department of Electronics and Electrical Engineering at Keio University. He received his PhD in electrical engineering from Keio University in 1990. His research interests have involved modeling of light propagation in biological tissue and its application to functional near infrared spectroscopy and diffuse optical imaging.

Yasuyo Minagawa is a professor in the Department of Psychology at Keio University. She received her PhD in medicine from the University of Tokyo in 2000. Her research examines the development of perception and cognition with a focus on language acquisition and social cognition in infants. To reveal the neural substrates underlying such development, she has successfully applied fNIRS to typically and atypically developing infants. This application extends to real-world neuroimaging such as mother-infant hyperscanning.

Hiroshi Kawaguchi is a senior research scientist at the Human Informatics and Interaction Research Institute, National Institute of Advanced Industrial Science and Technology, Japan. He received his PhD in engineering from Keio University, Japan. His current research interests include the development of human neuroimaging techniques, such as near-infrared spectroscopy.

CC BY: © The Authors. Published by SPIE under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Unported License. Distribution or reproduction of this work in whole or in part requires full attribution of the original publication, including its DOI.
Lin Cai, Eiji Okada, Yasuyo Minagawa, and Hiroshi Kawaguchi "Correlating functional near-infrared spectroscopy with underlying cortical regions of 0-, 1-, and 2-year-olds using theoretical light propagation analysis," Neurophotonics 8(2), 025009 (31 May 2021). https://doi.org/10.1117/1.NPh.8.2.025009
Received: 20 February 2021; Accepted: 18 May 2021; Published: 31 May 2021
Lens.org Logo
CITATIONS
Cited by 3 scholarly publications.
Advertisement
Advertisement
KEYWORDS
Brain

Head

Near infrared spectroscopy

Geometrical optics

Optical properties

Neurophotonics

Tissues

Back to Top